What are Conscious Sensations?

Authors

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7740150

Abstract

Existing theories about the nature of conscious sensations are discussed.  The oldest classification system contrasts dualist theories (which say consciousness is an abstract entity) with monist theories (which say consciousness is a concrete entity).  A more recent system contrasts process theories ("consciousness is a process, not a thing") with vehicle theories (consciousness is a property of one or more of the things associated with brain processes). The present paper first points out that processes are abstracta, which makes process theories dualist. It then argues that (a) dualist theories are untestable and therefore unscientific and (b) process theories which invoke information are at odds with the normal definition of information.  Then two separate kinds of vehicle theory are discussed: first the neural identity theory and then a theory that pulls together the enormous volume of data generated by Crick's suggestion to forget about theories and simply measure the neural correlates of consciousness into a proposal equating sensory consciousness with certain patterns in the electromagnetic fields generated by brain function.  The paper concludes with an injunction to stop researching this topic altogether, on the grounds that the results are likely to be used in unacceptably dystopian developments.

Keywords:

monism, dualism, process theories, vehicle theories, neural identity theory, NCCs, CEMF theory

Downloads

Download data is not yet available.

Metrics

Metrics Loading ...

References

Atkinson AP, Thomas MSC and Cleeremans A. Consciousness: mapping the theoretical landscape. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2000; 4(10): 372-382

Aru J, Bachmann T, Singer W and Melloni L. Distilling the neural correlates of consciousness. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2012; 36: 737-746.

Barrie JM, Freeman WJ & Lenhart MD. Spatiotemporal analysis of prepyriform, visual, auditory, and somesthetic surface EEGs in trained rabbits. Journal of Neurophysiology 1996; 76(1): 520-39.

Barlow HB. Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology. Perception 1972; 1: 371-394.

Bédard C, Kröger H & Destexhe A. Modelling extracellular field potentials and the frequency-filtering properties of extracellular space, Biophysical Journal 2004; 86 (3):1829–1842.

Bédard C, Kröger H & Destexhe A. Modelling of low-pass filtering of local field potentials in brain tissue. Physical Review E 2006; 73: 051911

Berens P, Keliris GA, Ecker AS, Logothetis NK and Tolias AS. Feature selectivity of the gamma-band of the local field potential in primate primary visual cortex. Front Neurosci 2008; 2(2) doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.037.2008

Block N. On a confusion about a function of consciousness.Behav. Brain Sci 1995; 18:227–287.

Boyer JL, Harrison S and Ro T. Unconscious processing of orientation and color without primary visual cortex. PNAS 2005; 102:16875-16879.

Bremer F. Considérations sur l’origine et la nature des ‘ondes’ cérébrales. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1949; 1:177–193.

Buzsáki G, Anastassiou CA and Koch C. The origin of extracellular fields and currents – EEG, ECoG, LFP and spikes. Nature Rev Neurosci 2012; 13:408 – 420.

Chalmers DJ. The conscious mind: in search of a fundamental theory. New York, Oxford, OUP., 1996.

Connor CE. Friends and grandmothers. Nature 2005; 435:1036-1037.

Crick F. The astonishing hypothesis: the scientific search for the soul. New York: Simon and Schuster., 1994.

Davidson D. Mental events. In Essays on Actions and Events, D. Davidson OUP; Reprinted in Mind and Cognition Ed. WG Lycan (1990) Blackwell., 1980

Dehaene S and Naccache L. Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition 2001; 79:1–37.

Dehaene S and Changeux J-P. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron 2011; 70:200-227.

Del Cul A, Dehaene S, Reyes P, Bravo E, Slachevsky A. Causal role of prefrontal cortex in the threshold for access to consciousness. Brain 2009; 132:2531–2540.

de Graaf TA, de Jong MC, Goebel R, van E R, Sack AT. On the functional relevance of frontal cortex for passive and voluntarily controlled bistable vision. Cereb Cortex 2011; 21:2322–2331.

Desmurget M, Reilly KT, Richard N, Szathmari A, Mottolese C & Sirigu A. Movement intention after parietal cortex stimulation in humans. Science 2009; 324:811–813.

Eccles JC. Interpretation of action potentials evoked in the cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 1951; 3:449–464.

Einevoll GT, Kayser C, Logothetis NK and Panzeri S. Modelling and analysis of local field potentials for studying the function of cortical circuits. Nature Rev Neurosci 2013; 14: 770-785.

Engel AK, König P, Kreiter AK, Singer W. Interhemispheric synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses in cat visual cortex. Science 1991; 252:1177–9.

Engel AK, Kreiter AK, König P, Singer W. Synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses between striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas of the cat. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1991; 88:6048–52.

Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in topdown processing. Nat Rev 2001; 2:704-16.

Fahrenfort JJ, Scholte HS and Lamme VAF. Masking disrupts reentrant processing in human visual cortex. J Cog Neurosci 2007; 19:1488-1497.

Fein G, Raz J, Brown FF, Merrin EL. Common reference coherence data are confounded by power and phase effects. Electroenceph Clin Neurophys 1998; 69:581-4.

Feigl H. The ‘mental’ and the ‘physical’. Minnesota Studies in the Philosophy of Science Vol 2. Ed H. Feigl, G. Maxwell and M. Scriven. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press., 1958.

Festinger L. Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press., 1957.

Floridi L. Is semantic information meaningful data? Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 2005; 70(2):351- 370.

Freeman WJ. Spatial properties of an EEG event in the olfactory bulb and cortex. Electroencephalography & Clinical Neurophysiology 1978; 44(5): 586-605.

Freeman WJ. A cinematographic hypothesis ofcortical dynamics in perception. Int J Psychophysiol 2006; 60:149–161.

Freeman WJ & Baird B. Relation of olfactory EEG to behavior: spatial analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience 1987; 101(3): 393-408.

Freeman WJ & Grajski KA. Relation of olfactory EEG to behavior: factor analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience 1987; 101(6):766-77.

Freeman WJ & van Dijk BW. Spatial patterns of visual cortical fast EEG during conditioned reflex in a rhesus monkey. Brain Research 1987; 422(2): 267-76.

Fries P, Roelfsema PR, Engel AK, König P and Singer W. Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 1997; 94:12699–12704.

Goto T, Hatanaka R, Ogawa T, Sumiyoshi A, Riera J and Kawashima R. An evaluation of the conductivity profile in the somatosensory barrel cortex of Wistar rats. J Neurophysiol 2010; 104:3388-3412.

Gross CG. Genealogy of the “grandmother cell”. Neuroscientist 2002; 8(5):512-518.

Horton JC and Hedley-Whyte T. Mapping of cytochrome oxidase patches and ocular dominance columns in human visual cortex. Phil Trans Roy Soc Lond B 1984; 304: 255–72.

Huxley TH. Lessons in Elementary Physiology. London: MacMillan, 1866.

Jackson F. What Mary didn't know. Journal of Philosophy 1986; (83): 291-295.

James W. The principles of psychology. Holt, New York., 1890.

Jones EG. The Thalamus, 2nd ed., New York: Cambridge University Press., 2007.

Juan C.-H and Walsh V. Feedback to V1: a reverse hierarchy in vision. Exp Brain Res 2003; 150: 259-263.

Juan C.-H, Campana G and Walsh V. Cortical interactions in vision and awareness: hierarchies in reverse. Prog Brain Res 2004; 144:117-130.

Koivisto M, Mäntylä T and Silvanto J. The role of early visual cortex (V1/V2) in conscious and unconscious visual perception. Neuroimage 2010; 51:828-834.

Koch C. Ubiquitous minds. Scientific American Mind, Jan/Feb 2014; 26-29.

Lamme VAF. Separate neural definitions of visual consciousness and visual attention; a case for phenomenal awareness. Neural Networks 2004; 17:861-872.

Lamme VAF, Supèr H and Spekreijse H. Feedforward, horizontal and feedback processing in the visual cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998; 8:529-535.

Lamme VAF and Roelfsema P. The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing. Trends Neurosci 2000; 23:571-579.

Lamme VAF, Spekreijse H. Neuronal synchrony does not represent texture segregation. Nature 1998; 396:362–6.

Lee TS, Mumford D, Romero R and Lamme VAF. The role of primary visual cortex in higher level vision. Vision Research 1998; 38:2429-2454.

Lehmann D. Multichannel topography of human alpha EEG fields. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1971; 31:439–449.

Lehmann D and Koenig T. Spatio-temporal dynamics of alpha brain electric fields andcognitive modes. Int J Psychophysiol 1997; 26: 99–112.

Lehmann D, Ozaki H and Pal I. EEG alpha map series: brain micro-states by space-oriented adaptive segmentation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987; 67: 271–288.

Lehmann D, Strik WK, Henggeler B, Koenig T and Koukkou M. Brain electric microstates and momentary conscious mind states as building blocks of spontaneous thinking: I. Visual imagery and abstract thoughts. Int J Psychophysiol 1998; 29: 1–11.

Leopold DA & Logothetis NK. Activity changes in early visual cortex reflect monkeys' percepts during binocular rivalry. Nature 1996; 379:549-553.

Levine J. Materialism and qualia: The explanatory gap. Pacific Philosophical Quarterly 1983; 64:354-361

Libet B, Alberts WW, Wright EW, Delattre LD, Levin G & Feinstein B. Production of threshold levels of conscious sensation by electrical stimulation of human somatosensory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology 1964; 27:546–578.

Libet B, Gleason CA, Wright EW and Pearl DK. Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 1983; 106:623-642.

Logothetis NK & Schall JD. Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science 1989; 245:761-763.

Maier A, Wilke M, Aura C, Zhu C, Ye FQ and Leopold DA. Divergence of fMRI and neural signals in V1 during perceptual suppression in the awake monkey. Nat Neurosci 2008; 11:1193–1200.

McFadden JJ. The CEMI field theory: gestalt information and the meaning of meaning. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2013; 20:152-182.

Melloni L, Molina C, Pena M, Torres D, Singer W and Rodriguez E. Synchronization of neural activity across cortical areas correlates with conscious perception. J Neurosci 2007; 27(11): 2858-2856.

Pascual-Leone A and Walsh V. Fast back projections from the motion to the primary visual area necessary for visual awareness. Science 2001; 292:510-512.

Pettersen KH, Devor A, Ulbert I, Dale A and Einevoll GT. Current-source density estimation based on inversion of electrostatic forward solution: Effects of finite extent of neuronal activity and conductivity discontinuities. J Neurosci Methods 2006; 154:115-133.

Place UT. Is Consciousness a Brain Process? British Journal of Psychology 1956; 47:44–50.

Pockett S. The nature of consciousness: a hypothesis. iUniverse; Lincoln Nebraska, Shanghai, 2002, p.212

Pockett S. On subjective back-referral and how long it takes to become conscious of a stimulus: a reinterpretation of Libet’s data. Consciousness and Cognition 2002; 11:144-161.

Pockett S. The neuroscience of movement, in Pockett, S., Banks, W. and Gallagher, S. (eds.) Does Consciousness Cause Behavior? Cambridge, MA: MIT Press., 2006. pp. 9–24.

Pockett S. The electromagnetic field theory of consciousness: a testable hypothesis about the characteristics of conscious as opposed to non-conscious fields. Journal of Consciousness Studies 2012; 19(11-12):191-223.

Pockett S, Zhou ZZ, Brennan BJ and Bold GEJ. Spatial resolution and the neural correlates of sensory experience. Brain Topography 2007; 20:1-6.

Pockett S, Bold GEJ & Freeman WJ. EEG synchrony during a perceptual-cognitive task: Widespread phase synchrony at all frequencies. Clinical Neurophysiology 2009; 120:695–708.

Pockett S and Holmes MD. Intracranial EEG power spectra and phase synchrony during consciousness and unconsciousness. Consciousness and Cognition 2009; 18:1049-1055

Pockett S, Brennan BJ, Bold GEJ and Holmes MD. A possible physiological basis for the discontinuity of consciousness. Front Psychology 2011; 2:377. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00377

Pockett S and Purdy SC. Are voluntary movements initiated preconsciously? The relationships between readiness potentials, urges and decisions. In: Conscious Will and Responsibility: A Tribute to Benjamin Libet. W Sinnott-Armstrong and L Nadel (Eds). New York; OUP., 2011.

Pollen D. On the neural correlates of visual perception. Cerebral Cortex 1999; 9:4-19.

Pollen D. Explicit neural representations, recursive neural networks and conscious visual perception. Cerebral Cortex 2003; 13:807-814.

Pollen D. Fundamental requirements for primary visual perception. Cerebral Cortex 2008; 18:1991-1998.

Putnam H. Psychological predicates. In Art, Philosophy and Religion Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press, reprinted under the title “The nature of mental states” in various places, including The Nature of Mind ed D.M. Rosenthal, New York OUP., 1967/1991.

Quiroga RQ, Reddy L, Kreiman G, Koch C and Fried I. Invariant visual representation by single neurons in the human brain. Nature 2005; 435:1102-1107.

Ro T, Breitmeyer B, Burton P, Singhai NS and Lane D. Feedback contributions to visual awareness in human occipital cortex. Current Biology 2003; 14:1038-1041.

Rodriguez E, George N, Lachaux J-P, Martinerie J, Renault B and Varela FJ. Perception’s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature 1999; 397:430-433.

Roelsema PR, König P, Engel AK, Sireteanu R and Singer W. Reduced synchronization in the visual cortex of cats with strabismic amblyopia. European Journal of Neuroscience 1994; 6:1645-1655.

Roelfsema PR, Engel AK, König P & Singer W. Visuomotor integration is associated with zero time-lag synchronization among cortical areas. Nature 1997; 385:157-161.

Rubio-Garrido P, Pérez-de-Manzo F, Porrero C, Galazo MJ & Clascá F. Thalamic input to distal apical dendrites in neocortical layer 1 is massive and highly convergent, Cerebral Cortex 2009; 19:2380–2395.

Sayre KM. Cybernetics and the philosophy of mind. Routledge and Kegan Paul, London., 1976.

Seth AK, Barrett AB and Barnett L. Causal density and integrated information as measures of conscious level. Phil Trans Roy Soc A 2011; 369:3748-3767.

Shadlen MN, Movshen JA. Synchrony unbound: a critical evaluation of the temporal binding hypothesis. Neuron 1999; 24:67–77.

Shannon CE. A mathematical theory of communication. The Bell System Technical Journal 1948; 27: 379-423.

Sheinberg DL & Logothetis NK. The role of temporal cortical areas in perceptual organization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 1997; 94: 3408-3413.

Silvanto J, Lavie N and Walsh V. Double dissociation of V1 and V5.MT activity in visual awareness. Cerebral Cortex 2005; 15:1736-1741.

Singer W and Gray CM. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis, Annu Rev Neurosci 1995; 18:555–586.

Smart JJC. Sensations and brain processes. Philosophical Review 1959; 68:141-156.

Smart JJC. The Mind/Brain Identity Theory. The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2012 Edition), Edward N. Zalta (ed.), URL = http://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2012/entries/mind-identity. 2012.

Steriade M, Amzica F and Contreras D. Synchronization of fast 30–40 Hz spontaneous cortical rhythms during brain activation. J Neurosci 1996; 16:392–417.

Supèr H, Spekreijse H and Lamme VAF. Two distinct modes of sensory processing observed in monkey primary visual cortex (V1). Nature Neurosci 2001; 4(3):304-310.

Tononi G. An information integration theory of consciousness. BMC Neuroscience 2004; 5:42. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-5-42

Tononi G. Consciousness as integrated information: a provisional manifesto. Biological Bulletin 2008; 215:216-242.

Tononi G (2012) Integrated information theory of consciousness: an updated account. Archives Italiennes de Biologie 150:290-326.

Tononi G and Edelman GM. Consciousness and complexity. Science 1998; 282:1846-1851.

Tononi G, Srinivasan R, Russell DP and Edelman GM. Investigating neural correlates of conscious perception by frequency-tagged neuromagnetic responses. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 1998; 95:3198-3203.

Trujillo LT, Peterson MA, Kaszniak AW & Allen JJB. EEG phase synchrony differences across visual perception conditions may depend on recording and analysis methods. Clinical Neurophysiology 2005; 116:172–189.

Tse PU, Martinez-Conde S, Schlegel AA, Macknik SL. Visibility, visual awareness, and visual masking of simple unattended targets are confined to areas in the occipital cortex beyond human V1/V2. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005; 102:17178–17183.

Vanderwolf CH. Are neocortical gamma waves related to consciousness? Brain Res 2000; 855: 217-224.

Velmans M. Is human information processing conscious? Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1991; 14:651-669.

Wilke M, Logothetis NK and Leopold DA. Local field potential reflects perceptual suppression in monkey visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2006; 103:17507–17512.

Downloads

Published

16.03.2023

How to Cite

Pockett , S. (2023). What are Conscious Sensations?. Journal of NeuroPhilosophy, 2(1). https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.7740150